Wednesday, 24 October 2018

What would you be without your memory?







Image result for brain
The Human Brain
  

Can you imagine what you would be without your memory? How would you feel and what would 
you do if you woke up one morning and realized you could not remember the food you ate and what you did the previous night before sleeping?  What if you can't recognize your spouse, or remember how to wear your shirt or eat your favourite meal? What if you can't find your way to that club you love to visit?  Would the world still make sense to you?



Our memory makes us who we are. It gives us our identities, and enables us to know our family and the environment we live in. If we can’t remember our name, what we did, when and where we did it, and if we can’t remember our environment, then we have lost our identity. Also, it will be extremely difficult for us to learn new things. We learn and relive our past experiences, even childhood experiences, because of the amazing power to memorize things. Our memory allows us to learn new habits and unlearn old nasty ones, and also develop relationships. These abilities to learn new things, remember past events and develop relationships are among the most basic powers of our brain. 

What is memory?
Memory is the ability of the brain to encode, store and retrieve information. It is the preservation of information over a period of time in order to influence future action. It is involved in processing vast amounts of information. This information takes many different forms, e.g. images, sounds or meaning.

What are the stages of memory ? The three main stages of memory are encoding, storage and retrieval

Stages of memory

Encoding: When a piece of information or a sensory impulse enters our memory system, it has to be changed into a form that the brain can handle, so that it can be stored.  Think of this process as similar to changing your money into a different currency when you travel from one country to another.  For example, a word seen in a book may be stored if it is changed (encoded) into a sound or a meaning (i.e. semantic processing). In addition, studies have revealed that attention greatly affects how information is encoded much more than how fast it is recalled. Hence, “distractions or divided attention during initial learning may severely impair subsequent retrieval success, whereas distractions during the recall of information may impede the process a little, but has no effect on its accuracy.”.
 
There are three main ways the brain can encode (change) information that it receives:

1.Visual (picture)

2. Acoustic (sound)
3. Semantic (meaning)
How do you remember a series of numbers you see in a book?  If you can see the numbers, then you are using the visual coding; but if you are repeating the numbers to yourself, you are using the acoustic coding. Also, research has shown that it is the acoustic coding that is employed in short-term memory (STM).  When a person is presented with a list of numbers and letters, they will try to hold them in the STM by rehearsing them (verbally). On the other hand, the semantic coding (coding by meaning) is the principle encoding system used in long-term memory (LTM). But information in the LTM can also be coded both visually and acoustically. 

Storage: This is another stage of memory which involves the nature of memory stores in the brain, i.e., where the information is stored, the duration of the memory, how much can be stored at any time (capacity), and what kind of information is stored.  How a piece of information is stored in the brain determines the way it is retrieved.  Most adults can store between 5 and 9 items in their short-term memory.   However, if we can chunk information together, then we can store a lot more of it in our short-term memory.  In contrast, the capacity of LTM is thought to be unlimited, as information can last a lifetime in the LTM, whereas in the STM, it can last for a about 0.3s.
Retrieval: This is yet another stage of memory that involves getting information out of the brain. Whenever we can’t recall a thing, then it means we can’t retrieve it from where it is stored in the brain. And the difference between the short and long term memory becomes evident when we try to retrieve information.
STM is stored and retrieved sequentially.  For example, if a list of words is read out to a group of participants, and then they are asked to recall the fourth word on the list, the participants will go through the list in the order they heard it in order to retrieve the information.
On the other hand, LTM is stored and retrieved by association.  This is why you can remember what you went upstairs for if you go back to the room where you first thought about it.
Organizing information can aid its retrieval.  You can organize information in sequences (such as alphabetically, by size or by time).

What are the types of memory
Types of human memory
  

The three main types of human memory are the sensory, short-term and long-term memory. The long-term memory is then divided into explicit (declarative) memory and implicit (procedural) memory (Types of memory, n.d.).   

“The sensory and short-term memories must be worked through before a long-term memory can be established. And each of the three main types of memory has its own particular mode of operation, but they all cooperate in the process of memorization. This model of memory as a sequence of three stages, from sensory to short-term to long-term memory, rather than as a unitary process, is known as the modal (multi-store) or Atkinson-Shiffrin model, after Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin who developed it in 1968.”
Sensory Memory: The sensory memory is a transient element of memory. It is the ability to retain impressions of sensory information after the original stimuli have ended. It acts as a kind of buffer for stimuli received through the five senses of sight, hearing, smell, taste and touch, which are retained accurately, but very briefly. For example, the ability to look at something and remember what it looked like with just a second of observation is an example of sensory memory
Short-term Memory: The short-term memory is also known as the working memory. It acts as a kind of “scratch-pad” for temporary recall of the information which is being processed at any point in time. It can be thought of as the ability to remember and process information at the same time.
Long -term Memory: The long-term memory is intended for storage of information over a long period of time. Even though we forget things everyday, it seems that the long-term memory actually fades very little over time, which is why it can store a seemingly unlimited amount of information almost indefinitely. 

Short-term memories can become long-term memory through the process of consolidation, involving rehearsal and meaningful association. But unlike the short-term memory (which relies mostly on an acoustic, and to a lesser extent, a visual code for storing information), long-term memory encodes information for storage semantically (i.e. based on meaning and association).

Explicit Memory: The explicit memory, also known as the declarative memory is the conscious, intentional recollection of facts, previous experiences and concepts. The explicit memory is divided into episodic and semantic memory. Episodic memory consists of the storage and recollection of observational information attached to specific life-events. These can be memories of personal experiences or memories of events that happened around a person. Episodic memory allows a person to mentally travel back in time – recalling various contextual and situational details of one's previous experiences. On the other hand, semantic memory refers to knowledge of facts, ideas, meaning and concepts that can be articulated, and is independent of personal experience. This includes world knowledge, object knowledge, language knowledge, and conceptual priming.

Implicit memory: the implicit memory or procedural memory refers to unconscious memories such as skills (e.g. knowing how to get dressed, eat, drive, and ride a bicycle without having to re-learn the skill each time). The implicit memory allows us perform a regular task or a habit without thinking about the procedure.
The other types of memory include autobiographical memory and the spatial memory. The autobiographical memory is a memory system that consists of episodes recollected from an individual's life, based on a combination of episodic (personal experiences and specific objects, people and events experienced at particular time and place) and semantic (general knowledge and facts about the world) memory.

The spatial memory is the part of memory that records information about one's environment and its spatial orientation. A person's spatial memory is needed in order to navigate around a familiar city. For example, you use your spatial memory to locate your friend's house, find the ATM when you need some cash, and locate a restaurant when you are hungry. Research has shown that there are specific areas of the brain, such as the hippocampus, that are associated with spatial memory.

What are the brain structures involved in memory
Mid-saggital section of the brain showing the hippocampus and some structures in the basal ganglia and limbic system.

There are three most important brain structures involved in memory. They include the hippocampus, lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and amygdala. However, the hippocampus and its surrounding structures are the most important in declarative memory (Eichenbaum, 2001).

The other regions of the brain that play a role in memory include regions of the diencephalon, which are activated when a remote memory is being recalled (Graham & Levine, 2004). In addition, the occipital lobe, medial temporal lobe, and fusiform gyrus all play a role in memory formation (Gabrieli & Kao, 2007).
 Hippocampus: The hippocampus is essential for memory function, particularly the transfer of information from short to long-term memory. The hippocampus is also responsible for the control of spatial memory and behavior (Parts of the brain, n.d). It also has a great ability to retain and recall episodic memories (Eichenbaum, 2001). Moreover, the formation of new declarative memories relies on both the hippocampus and parahippocampus (Gabrieli & Kao, 2007)

Amygdala
  
                                                                                The amygdala

The amygdala performs a primary role in the processing and storing of emotional reactions, social and sexual behaviour, as well as regulating the sense of smell (Parts of the brain, n.d). It is believed to be involved in the encoding and retrieval of emotionally charged memories, such as the Biafra War and the cancellation of the results of the 1993 presidential elections. Such emotional memories have been linked to increased activity in the amygdala (Sharot et al, 2007).

Lateral Prefrontal Cortex: The lateral prefrontal cortex plays an important part in processing short-term memories and retaining long-term memories. It is also essential for remembering contextual details of an experience rather than for memory formation (Parts of the brain, n.d; Gabrieli & Kao, 2007). The PFC is also more involved with episodic memory than semantic memory, although it does play a small role in semantics (Graham & Levine, 2004). 

What are the factors that affect declarative memory?
The factors that affect declarative memory include stress and neurochemical factors such as cortisol. When subjected to a stressful condition, an individual might come down with decreased declarative memory performance (Lupien et al, 1997).
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) emerges after exposure to a traumatic event eliciting fear, horror or helplessness that involves bodily injury, the threat of injury, or death to one's self or another person (Cabeza & LaBar, 2006). The chronic stress in PTSD contributes to an observed decrease in hippocampal volume and declarative memory deficits (Baker et al, 2005).
Cortisol is the primary glucocorticoid in the human body. In the brain, it modulates the ability of the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex to process memories (Damoiseaux & Elzinga, 2007). 

Recent studies on memory
A recent study by Dal Maso et al showed that cardiovascular exercise performed immediately after practicing a visual-motor tracking task has been shown to improve the long-term retention of this motor skill through an optimization of the memory consolidation process. After the experiment, the researchers identified potential neural substrates through which a single bout of acute exercise, when performed in close temporal proximity to motor practice, strengthens motor memories. In another study, it was discovered that sleep boosts memory in young adults (Jones et al, 2018).
Sleep plays an active role in the consolidation of declarative memory, and the mechanism by which it does this is by reactivating hippocampal memory representations. This reactivation transfers information to neocortical networks where it is integrated into long-term representations (McClelland et al, 1995).
Examples of memory disorders
Memory disorders occur as a result of damage to neuroanatomical structures, leading to impairment in the storage, retention and recollection of memories. Memory disorders can either be progressive, as in the case of Alzheimer’s disease, or immediate as in the case of a head injury. Memory disorders include:
  • Agnosia
  • Alzheimer's disease
  • Amnesia
  • Brain injury
  • Dementia
  • Hyperthymestic syndrome
  • Huntington's disease
  • Parkinson's disease
  • Stress
  • Wernicke- Korsakoff's syndrome


Sources:
1.      Memory (n.d.). Psychology Today Site. Retrieved from: https://www.psychologytoday.com/intl/basics/memory

Sherwood, L. (2015, January 1). Human Physiology: From Cells to Systems. Cengage Learning. pp. 157–162. ISBN 978-1-305-44551-2

3.      McLeod, S. A. (2007). Stages of memory - encoding storage and retrieval. Retrieved from https://www.simplypsychology.org/memory.html
4.      Types of Memory (n.d). The Human Memory Site. Retrieved from: http://www.human-  memory.net/types.html
5.      Ullman, MT (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition. 92: 231–70. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008
6.      McRae, Ken & Jones, Michael (2013). Reisberg, Daniel, ed. The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. pp. 206–216. ISBN 9780195376746.
7.      Williams, H. L., Conway, M. A., & Cohen, G. (2008). Autobiographical memory. In G. Cohen & M. A. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the Real World (3rd ed., pp. 21-90). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
8.      Eichenbaum, Howard (2001). "The Hippocampus and Declarative Memory: Cognitive Mechanisms and Neural Codes". Behavioural Brain Research. 127: 199–207. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00365-5
9.      Graham, S.; Levine, B. (2004). "The Fundamental Neuroanatomy of Episodic and Semantic Autobiographical Remembering: A Prospective Functional MRI Study". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 16: 1633–1646. doi:10.1162/0898929042568587
10.  Gabrieli, J.; Kao, Y. (2007). "Development of the Declarative Memory System in the Human Brain". Nature Neuroscience. 10: 1198–1205. doi:10.1038/nn1950
11.  Parts f the brain (n.d). The Human Memory Site. Retrieved from: http://www.human-memory.net/brain_parts.html
12.  Sharot, T; Martorella, EA; Delgado, MR; Phelps, EA (2007). "How personal experience modulates the neural circuitry of memories of September 11". Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 104: 389–394. doi:10.1073/pnas.0609230103.
13.  Dal Maso, Fabien, et al (2018). "Acute cardiovascular exercise promotes functional changes in cortico-motor networks during the early stages of motor memory consolidation." NeuroImage 174: 380-392.
14.  Lupien, S., Gaudreau, S., Tchiteya, B., Maheu, F., Sharma, S., Nair, N. et al. (1997). "Stress-Induced Declarative Memory Impairment in Healthy Elderly Subjects: Relationship to Cortisol Reactivity". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 82 (7): 2070–2075. doi:10.1210/jc.82.7.2070
15.  Cabeza, R.; LaBar, K. S. (2006). "Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory". 7: 54–64
16.  Baker, D. G.; et al. (2005). "Higher levels of basal CSF cortisol in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J". Psychiatry. 162: 992–994. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.992
17.  Damoiseaux, J.S.; Elzinga, B.M. (2007). "Glucocorticoids Decrease Hippocampal and Prefrontal Activation during Declarative Memory Retrieval in Young Men". Brain Imaging and Behaviour. 1: 31–41
18.   Jones, B.J., Mackay, A., Kurt, S. S., Spenser, R.M.C., (2018). The role of sleep in emotional memory processing in middle age. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.  155: 208-215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2018.08.002
19.  McClelland, J.L.; McNaughton, B.L.; O'Reilly, R.C. (1995). "Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory". Psychol. Rev. 102: 419–457. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.3.419




 


No comments:

Post a Comment